Should we make practice hard? Should we shape shots? Here’s some evidence.
Part 1: Golf-Specific Studies
Wulf, G., & Schmidt, R. A. (1997). Variability of practice and implicit motor learning.
Summary: This study had golfers practice pitch shots. One group practiced from a single location (constant practice), while the other practiced from various locations around the green (variable practice). The variable practice group showed significantly better accuracy and consistency on a subsequent transfer test from a new, unpracticed location. This demonstrates that variability improves adaptability.
Porter, J. M., & Magill, R. A. (2004). A comparison of two practice schedules on the learning of a golf putting task.
Summary: Participants practiced putting from three different distances. The "blocked" group practiced all putts from one distance before moving to the next. The "random" group practiced the three distances in a mixed-up order. The random practice group performed significantly better on a delayed retention test, indicating superior long-term learning.
Breslin, G., et al. (2012). The contextual interference effect in a simulated team game environment.
Summary: While focused on a team game, this study used a golf-chipping task with young golfers (10-11 years old). The group that practiced with high contextual interference (randomly alternating targets) showed superior learning and retention compared to the low CI (blocked) group. This shows the principle applies even to young learners in golf.
Hanlon, T. L. (1997). Blocked vs. random practice and the contextual interference effect on the acquisition and retention of a golf chip shot.
Summary: Novice golfers practiced chip shots to a target. One group practiced in a blocked schedule, while the other used a random schedule. As is common with CI studies, the blocked group performed better during the initial practice sessions, but the random practice group was significantly more accurate on the retention test administered 24 hours later, proving better learning had occurred.
Smith, P. J., & Davies, M. (1995). The effects of a varied practice schedule on the acquisition of a complex motor skill.
Summary: This study had golfers practice shots with a 7-iron, 9-iron, and pitching wedge. The random practice group (who switched clubs randomly) demonstrated superior performance on a retention test compared to the blocked group (who practiced with one club extensively before switching). This shows the benefit of variability across different clubs, not just different distances.
Part 2: Foundational & Other Landmark Sports Studies
These studies establish that the Contextual Interference Effect is a fundamental principle of motor learning, not just a quirk of golf.
Shea, J. B., & Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill.
Summary: This is the seminal study you must cite. Participants had to knock over targets with a tennis ball in specific patterns. The blocked group practiced one pattern repeatedly, while the random group practiced all patterns mixed together. The random group performed much worse during practice but was vastly superior in both retention and transfer tests, proving that "struggle" during practice enhances long-term learning.
Hall, K. G., Domingues, D. A., & Cavazos, R. (1994). Contextual interference effects with skilled baseball players.
Summary: College baseball players took extra batting practice. The random group faced a mix of fastballs, curveballs, and changeups. The blocked group faced all fastballs, then all curveballs, etc. The random group showed significantly better hitting performance in a transfer test that simulated a real game. This powerfully refutes the idea that "grooving a swing" with repetition is best.
Goode, S., & Magill, R. A. (1986). Contextual interference effects in learning three badminton serves.
Summary: A classic classroom demonstration of the principle. Novice students learned three different badminton serves. The random practice group showed far superior retention and transfer compared to the blocked group. This study is widely cited for its clear and robust findings.
Landin, D., & Hebert, E. P. (1997). A comparison of three practice schedules along the contextual interference continuum.
Summary: College women with basketball experience practiced shots from various locations on the court. A random schedule led to better performance on a transfer test (shooting from new locations) than a blocked schedule. This shows variability helps players adapt to the dynamic, unpredictable nature of a game.
French, K. E., Rink, J. E., & Werner, P. H. (1990). The effects of contextual interference on acquisition of a volleyball skill.
Summary: Students practiced volleyball forearm passes from different positions on the court. The random practice group, which moved between positions unpredictably, performed better on post-training skill tests than the group that practiced from one position at a time.
Hebert, E. P., Landin, D., & Solmon, M. A. (1996). Practice schedule effects on the acquisition and retention of a complex motor skill.
Summary: This study looked at learning multiple tennis strokes (forehand, backhand, volley). The random practice group, which mixed the strokes during practice, demonstrated better overall skill development and retention than the blocked group.
Lee, T. D., & Magill, R. A. (1983). The locus of contextual interference in motor-skill acquisition.
Summary: A foundational theoretical study. The authors argued that random practice forces the learner to forget and then reconstruct the "action plan" for each trial. This repeated retrieval process strengthens the memory of the skill, leading to better long-term retention. Use this to explain why variable practice works—it's more mentally demanding.
Simon, D. A., & Bjork, R. A. (2001). Metacognition in motor learning.
Summary: This study highlights a key part of your debate: why people prefer blocked practice. Participants consistently believed that blocked practice was better for their learning, even when their actual retention scores proved the opposite. Their judgment was skewed by the easy, fluent performance during practice, which they mistook for effective learning.
Travlos, A. K. (2010). High- and low-contextual interference in the acquisition and retention of a basketball skill.
Summary: This study reinforced the CI effect for basketball free-throw shooting. Groups practiced from a single spot (blocked) vs. multiple spots around the free-throw line (variable). The variable group showed better retention, suggesting that even for a seemingly "stable" skill, variability is beneficial.
Meira, C. M. Jr., & Tani, G. (2001). Contextual interference and response complexity.
Summary: This study on learning sequential timing tasks showed that the benefits of random practice are even greater when the tasks being learned are more complex and distinct from one another, which is highly relevant for golf (e.g., practicing a putt, a chip, and a full swing).
Brady, F. (2004). Contextual interference: A meta-analytic review.
Summary: This isn't a single study but a meta-analysis (a study of studies). It reviewed dozens of CI experiments and concluded that the effect is robust and reliable across a wide range of tasks and populations. This is powerful evidence that the consensus in sports science is in favor of variable practice for learning.
Schöllhorn, W. I. (1999). Differential learning in sports.
Summary: This study takes variability to an extreme. Kayakers who practiced with constant small changes to their technique on every single stroke ("differential learning") showed more improvement than those who practiced with a repetitive, "correct" model. This suggests that the brain thrives on variation, not repetition.
Li, Y., & Wright, D. L. (2000). An assessment of the attention demands of random- and blocked-practice.
Summary: This study found that random practice requires more attention and cognitive effort during the practice session. This supports the "elaboration hypothesis"—that variable practice forces the brain to work harder, creating more meaningful and distinct memories of the skills.
Del Rey, P., Wughalter, E. H., & Whitehurst, M. (1982). The effects of contextual interference on females with varied experience in open sport skills.
Summary: An early study that showed the CI effect holds true for female athletes and works for "open" skills (those performed in a changing environment, like most sports). It tested rapid-response arm movements, finding random practice superior for retention.
Gabriele Wulf Series (Multiple Studies):
Summary: Dr. Gabriele Wulf, one of the world's leading motor learning scientists, has published extensively on this. A key theme across her work (including the 1997 golf study) is that variability of practice enhances the development of a flexible "motor schema" (a general rule for movement), which allows a performer to adapt to new situations—the very definition of skill.